The young and the restless

Elderly chinese men playing chess

It struck me recently that one of the key differences between economists and neuroscientists studying decision-making is their interest in dynamics.  Economists seem more interested in explaining how behavior operates (or should operate) on average whereas neuroscientists would like to explain trial-to-trial variability.  Decisions are rarely made just once in a lifetime, but are instead made repeatedly.  Any behaviorist would instantly tell you that this means that there will be a learning component, something that I hardly see in the economic decision-making literature (feel free to correct me if this is wrong).

In many of these repeated decisions, people are not simply making a decision in a vacuum but are responding to the actions of others.  The decision must then be balanced by their prior beliefs, the results of recent decisions, and their predictions of how other people will act.  All of this can be incorporated into a reinforcement learning (RL) paradigm, where the expected value of any action is a combination of classical RL – where every payoff suggests future payoffs, and every loss suggests future losses – as well as a ‘mentalizing’ component that predicts how the opponent is likely to act, and how the opponent will react.  By fitting the responses of different brain regions to this type of model, one can get a sense of what each region is (kind of) doing.  One region that instantly pops out is the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC): this region is highly correlated with the prediction of other people’s behavior.

I once took a behavioral economics class in which the professor pointed out that deviations from rational behavior are only important if they translate to something in aggregate.  In other words, who cares if just a few people have abnormal mPFC function.  In a large population you won’t notice them.  But in fact there is a very large group of people with degraded mPFC: the elderly.  13 percent of the US is over the age of 65, and this group is known to have significant loss of volume in mPFC.  The prediction, then, would be that older individuals would be less inclined to take into account the behavior of other individuals when making decisions.

To test how they will act, we can take the experimental game the “Patent Race”.  In this game, two players are selected from a pool to compete for a prize.  They are each given either a large five credit or a small four credit endowment, and are asked to “invest” some portion of that.  They then get to keep whatever is left over, and the person who “invested” the most wins ten extra credits.

Cumulative distribution plots of how influential other individual's behavior is in determining one's own behavior.  Blue represents young adults and purple-dashed represents the elderly.

Cumulative distribution plots of how influential other individual’s behavior is in determining one’s own behavior. Blue represents young adults and purple-dashed represents the elderly.

There does exist a Nash equilibria to this game, and young adults will play the Nash equilibria exactly.  Old adults, on the other hand, play a significantly different strategy.  What is more interesting, though, is half of elderly adults behave as if they did not care at all about the strategy of the other player.  In other words, they are making decisions using a pure reinforcement learning strategy where they only cared about payoffs, not about how the other player was going to act.  In contrast, no young adults played like this: they all took into account the strategy that the other player would use.

References

Hampton, A., Bossaerts, P., & O’Doherty, J. (2008). Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105 (18), 6741-6746 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0711099105

Zhu, L., Walsh, D., & Hsu, M. (2012). Neuroeconomic Measures of Social Decision-Making Across the Lifespan Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00128

Photo from

Posts for the week

Bonobo genome sequenced, secrets of sex soon to be unlocked

Beetles are good parents!  And they’re social and talk!

On dopamine and being old

EO Wilson says war is inevitable, that’s just the way we are, someone else disagrees

Vampire electronics on the way

More about our microbiome

On Kuhn.  I wish I could see more of a discussion on the Kuhnian conception of science versus what we actually do in Biology

What men and women focus on when watching porn.  Surprisingly safe for work.

About being an adolescent:

We know from many human functional MRI, or FMRI studies, that the social brain is a network of brain regions that is consistently activated whenever adults think about other people. There are about three different regions in the brain, one in medial prefrontal cortex, and two other regions in the temporal lobe: the posterior-superior temporal sulcus, and the anterior temporal cortex. It doesn’t really matter about the names, but the point is that that network of brain regions in adults is consistently active whenever you think about other people or think about interacting with other people, or think about their mental states or their emotions.

Adolescents use the same network, the social brain network, to a very similar extent, but what seems to happen is that activity shifts from the anterior region, the medial prefrontal cortex region, to the posterior, the anterior temporal cortex or the superior temple sulcus region, as they go through adolescence. In other words, when they’re thinking about other people, adolescents seem to be using this prefrontal cortex, right at the front region, more than adults do, and adults seem to be using the temporal regions more than adolescents do.